Journal article 1230 views
Recall termination in free recall
Jonathan F Miller,
Christoph Weidemann,
Michael J Kahana
Memory & Cognition, Volume: 40, Issue: 4, Pages: 540 - 550
Swansea University Author: Christoph Weidemann
Full text not available from this repository: check for access using links below.
DOI (Published version): 10.3758/s13421-011-0178-9
Abstract
Although much is known about the dynamics of memory search in the free recall task, relatively little is known about the factors related to recall termination. Reanalyzing individual trial data from 14 prior studies (1,079 participants in 28,015 trials) and defining termination as occurring when a f...
Published in: | Memory & Cognition |
---|---|
ISSN: | 0090-502X 1532-5946 |
Published: |
2012
|
Online Access: |
Check full text
|
URI: | https://cronfa.swan.ac.uk/Record/cronfa7908 |
first_indexed |
2013-07-23T11:59:46Z |
---|---|
last_indexed |
2019-06-12T19:18:32Z |
id |
cronfa7908 |
recordtype |
SURis |
fullrecord |
<?xml version="1.0"?><rfc1807><datestamp>2019-06-12T15:39:45.7390905</datestamp><bib-version>v2</bib-version><id>7908</id><entry>2012-02-20</entry><title>Recall termination in free recall</title><swanseaauthors><author><sid>b155eeefe08155214e70fea25649223c</sid><ORCID/><firstname>Christoph</firstname><surname>Weidemann</surname><name>Christoph Weidemann</name><active>true</active><ethesisStudent>false</ethesisStudent></author></swanseaauthors><date>2012-02-20</date><deptcode>PSYS</deptcode><abstract>Although much is known about the dynamics of memory search in the free recall task, relatively little is known about the factors related to recall termination. Reanalyzing individual trial data from 14 prior studies (1,079 participants in 28,015 trials) and defining termination as occurring when a final response is followed by a long nonresponse interval, we observed that termination probability increased throughout the recall period and that retrieval was more likely to terminate following an error than following a correct response. Among errors, termination probability was higher following prior-list intrusions and repetitions than following extralist intrusions. To verify that this pattern of results can be seen in a single study, we report a new experiment in which 80 participants contributed recall data from a total of 9,122 trials. This experiment replicated the pattern observed in the aggregate analysis of the prior studies.</abstract><type>Journal Article</type><journal>Memory & Cognition</journal><volume>40</volume><journalNumber>4</journalNumber><paginationStart>540</paginationStart><paginationEnd>550</paginationEnd><publisher/><issnPrint>0090-502X</issnPrint><issnElectronic>1532-5946</issnElectronic><keywords/><publishedDay>31</publishedDay><publishedMonth>12</publishedMonth><publishedYear>2012</publishedYear><publishedDate>2012-12-31</publishedDate><doi>10.3758/s13421-011-0178-9</doi><url>http://cogsci.info/papers/MillerEtAl2012.pdf</url><notes/><college>COLLEGE NANME</college><department>Psychology School</department><CollegeCode>COLLEGE CODE</CollegeCode><DepartmentCode>PSYS</DepartmentCode><institution>Swansea University</institution><apcterm/><lastEdited>2019-06-12T15:39:45.7390905</lastEdited><Created>2012-02-20T23:57:58.3900000</Created><path><level id="1">Faculty of Medicine, Health and Life Sciences</level><level id="2">School of Psychology</level></path><authors><author><firstname>Jonathan F</firstname><surname>Miller</surname><order>1</order></author><author><firstname>Christoph</firstname><surname>Weidemann</surname><orcid/><order>2</order></author><author><firstname>Michael J</firstname><surname>Kahana</surname><order>3</order></author></authors><documents/><OutputDurs/></rfc1807> |
spelling |
2019-06-12T15:39:45.7390905 v2 7908 2012-02-20 Recall termination in free recall b155eeefe08155214e70fea25649223c Christoph Weidemann Christoph Weidemann true false 2012-02-20 PSYS Although much is known about the dynamics of memory search in the free recall task, relatively little is known about the factors related to recall termination. Reanalyzing individual trial data from 14 prior studies (1,079 participants in 28,015 trials) and defining termination as occurring when a final response is followed by a long nonresponse interval, we observed that termination probability increased throughout the recall period and that retrieval was more likely to terminate following an error than following a correct response. Among errors, termination probability was higher following prior-list intrusions and repetitions than following extralist intrusions. To verify that this pattern of results can be seen in a single study, we report a new experiment in which 80 participants contributed recall data from a total of 9,122 trials. This experiment replicated the pattern observed in the aggregate analysis of the prior studies. Journal Article Memory & Cognition 40 4 540 550 0090-502X 1532-5946 31 12 2012 2012-12-31 10.3758/s13421-011-0178-9 http://cogsci.info/papers/MillerEtAl2012.pdf COLLEGE NANME Psychology School COLLEGE CODE PSYS Swansea University 2019-06-12T15:39:45.7390905 2012-02-20T23:57:58.3900000 Faculty of Medicine, Health and Life Sciences School of Psychology Jonathan F Miller 1 Christoph Weidemann 2 Michael J Kahana 3 |
title |
Recall termination in free recall |
spellingShingle |
Recall termination in free recall Christoph Weidemann |
title_short |
Recall termination in free recall |
title_full |
Recall termination in free recall |
title_fullStr |
Recall termination in free recall |
title_full_unstemmed |
Recall termination in free recall |
title_sort |
Recall termination in free recall |
author_id_str_mv |
b155eeefe08155214e70fea25649223c |
author_id_fullname_str_mv |
b155eeefe08155214e70fea25649223c_***_Christoph Weidemann |
author |
Christoph Weidemann |
author2 |
Jonathan F Miller Christoph Weidemann Michael J Kahana |
format |
Journal article |
container_title |
Memory & Cognition |
container_volume |
40 |
container_issue |
4 |
container_start_page |
540 |
publishDate |
2012 |
institution |
Swansea University |
issn |
0090-502X 1532-5946 |
doi_str_mv |
10.3758/s13421-011-0178-9 |
college_str |
Faculty of Medicine, Health and Life Sciences |
hierarchytype |
|
hierarchy_top_id |
facultyofmedicinehealthandlifesciences |
hierarchy_top_title |
Faculty of Medicine, Health and Life Sciences |
hierarchy_parent_id |
facultyofmedicinehealthandlifesciences |
hierarchy_parent_title |
Faculty of Medicine, Health and Life Sciences |
department_str |
School of Psychology{{{_:::_}}}Faculty of Medicine, Health and Life Sciences{{{_:::_}}}School of Psychology |
url |
http://cogsci.info/papers/MillerEtAl2012.pdf |
document_store_str |
0 |
active_str |
0 |
description |
Although much is known about the dynamics of memory search in the free recall task, relatively little is known about the factors related to recall termination. Reanalyzing individual trial data from 14 prior studies (1,079 participants in 28,015 trials) and defining termination as occurring when a final response is followed by a long nonresponse interval, we observed that termination probability increased throughout the recall period and that retrieval was more likely to terminate following an error than following a correct response. Among errors, termination probability was higher following prior-list intrusions and repetitions than following extralist intrusions. To verify that this pattern of results can be seen in a single study, we report a new experiment in which 80 participants contributed recall data from a total of 9,122 trials. This experiment replicated the pattern observed in the aggregate analysis of the prior studies. |
published_date |
2012-12-31T18:15:54Z |
_version_ |
1821339764612136960 |
score |
11.04748 |